, 2006, Lefort et al , 2009 and Yoshimura et al , 2005) The conn

, 2006, Lefort et al., 2009 and Yoshimura et al., 2005). The connectivity between interneurons and principal cells has also been explored especially in the neocortex, where the large diversity of interneuron types suggests functional diversity. These studies generally report a cell-type-specific organization between cortical layers ( Jiang et al., 2013, Kätzel et al., 2011, Yoshimura and Callaway, 2005 and Yoshimura et al., 2005), but a dense nonspecific local connectivity ( Fino and Yuste, 2011 and Packer

and Yuste, 2011). The connectivity from excitatory to inhibitory cells ( Bock et al., 2011 and Hofer et al., 2011) suggests that cortical interneurons sample their excitatory inputs randomly. The available results thus indicate that interconnectivity of principal cells is structured, whereas connectivity of interneurons is unstructured. However, an important BAY 73-4506 in vitro element remains to be probed in more detail: the higher-order connectivity among interneurons. Recently, the interaction between the

different types of cortical interneurons and its functional implications have attracted interest ( Jiang et al., 2013, Letzkus et al., 2011 and Pi et al., 2013). Interneuron networks are known to share electrical and/or chemical synapses in various brain areas ( Bartos et al., 2002, Galarreta and Hestrin, 1999, Galarreta and Hestrin, 2002, Gibson et al., 1999, Landisman et al., 2002 and Tamás et al., Vorinostat 2000), including in a cell-type-specific manner ( Blatow et al., 2003, Gibson et al., 1999, Jiang et al., 2013 and Koós and Tepper, 1999) and are thought to underlie important features of network dynamics, such as synchronization and oscillations ( Bartos et al., 2007 and Whittington and Traub, 2003). However, quantitative information about the connectivity motifs and network architecture of interneuron-interneuron connections,

in particular among interneurons of the same cell type, is still lacking and is essential in order to fully understand their operation ( Buzsáki et al., 2004). Molecular layer interneurons in the cerebellum play an important role in regulating cerebellar output and motor learning (Jörntell et al., 2010). They are interconnected by GABAergic chemical synapses (Häusser and Clark, 1997 and Llano and Gerschenfeld, 1993) and by electrical synapses CYTH4 (Alcami and Marty, 2013 and Mann-Metzer and Yarom, 1999). The connections between molecular layer interneurons have important functional roles: the electrical connections can promote synchrony (Mann-Metzer and Yarom, 1999), whereas the chemical synapses can delay action potentials and affect the precision of spike timing in postsynaptic interneurons (Häusser and Clark, 1997 and Mittmann et al., 2005). However, the level of overlap between the chemical and electrical networks and their higher-level organization remain unclear.

This vertical shift of the contrast-discrimination

functi

This vertical shift of the contrast-discrimination

functions was reflected in smooth function fits to the data (Figure 3, solid curves; see Experimental Procedures: Psychophysical Contrast-Discrimination Functions) in which the parameter controlling vertical offset increased significantly for the distributed cue compared to the focal cue trials (gr, p = 0.02, Student’s t test across observers), but other parameters did not change (gc, p = 0.58; s, p = 0.17; q, p = 0.4, Student’s t test). Enhanced contrast discrimination could not be attributed to any change in eye position between focal and distributed cue trials (see Experimental Procedures: Eye Position Monitoring). Contrast-response functions were measured, in each of several MDV3100 visual cortical areas, for each of four

stimulus cue combinations (Figure 4): focal cue target, focal cue nontarget, distributed cue target, and distributed cue nontarget. fMRI responses increased monotonically with stimulus contrast (Figures 4A and 4B, representative observer), and depended on the stimulus cue combination (Figures 4B and 4C, representative observer and average, respectively). Response amplitudes were smallest for unattended stimuli (Figure 4C, green, focal cue nontarget), larger when attention was distributed http://www.selleckchem.com/screening/anti-cancer-compound-library.html (purple, distributed cue nontarget; blue, distributed cue target), and largest for attended targets (red, focal cue target). There was no evidence for response enhancement in any of the visual cortical areas. We fit the data by adopting a parametric equation for the contrast-response

functions (see Experimental Procedures: fMRI Contrast-Response Functions). Only one of the fitted parameter values differed significantly across the four stimulus cue combinations: the baseline response (b) that determined the vertical positions of the contrast-response functions. Allowing only this parameter to vary across cue conditions provided a fit that was statistically indistinguishable from the fit allowing next all parameters (gc, gr, and b) to vary across cue conditions (V1, V2, V3, and hV4 each F(14,8), p = 0.3). Thus, we did not observe a change in gain or slope of the contrast-response functions, consistent with previous reports ( Figure 1B; Buracas and Boynton, 2007 and Murray, 2008). Instead, the cue effect was well described as a vertical additive shift of the contrast-response functions. The amount of additive offset increased across the hierarchy of visual cortical areas. Values for b increased from the focal cue nontarget curve to the distributed cue nontarget curve by 0.04, 0.08, 0.14, and 0.25 (percent [%] fMRI signal change) in visual areas V1, V2, V3, and hV4, respectively. The values increased from focal cue nontarget to distributed cue nontarget by 0.11, 0.18, 0.27 and 0.34, and they increased from focal cue nontarget to focal cue target by 0.29, 0.39, 0.52, and 0.51.

For inactivation

of AlstR-expressing neurons, the peptide

For inactivation

of AlstR-expressing neurons, the peptide ligand AL (Ser-Arg-Pro-Tyr-Ser-Phe-Gly-Leu-NH2) was applied by perfusion. Organotypic brain slice cultures were used for testing SADΔG-GFP-rtTA. After biolistic transfection of both pCMMP-TVA800 and pTetO-CMVmin-Histone2B-mCherry-F2A-B19G, slices were infected with EnvA-SADΔG-GFP-rtTA and maintained in the selleck products absence or presence of dox (1.0 μg/ml). For testing SADΔG-GFP-ERT2CreERT2, HEK293t cells were transfected with the Cre-dependent plasmid pCALNL-DsRed, infected with the rabies virus, and maintained in the absence or presence of 4-HOT (1.0 μM). For testing SADΔG-FLPo-DsRedX, HeLa cells stably expressing a frt-STOP-frt nuclear-localized LacZ cassette were infected with the virus and then processed R428 in vitro for X-gal staining. Full methods are available in Supplemental Information. We thank I.R. Wickersham and J. Choi for helpful discussions; K.D. Roby, M. De La Parra, and K. von Bochmann for technical assistance; members of the Callaway laboratory for stimulating discussions; K.K. Conzelmann for the

BSR T7/5 cell line; O. Britz and M. Goulding for the HeLa cells expressing frt-STOP-frt-nLacZ; I.M. Verma for HIV lentivirus packaging plasmids; X. Wu for the pNLST7; R. Tsien for the mCherry plasmid; K. Deisseroth for the ChR2-mCherry plasmid; L.L. Looger for the GCaMP3 plasmid; and C.L. Cepko for the pCAG-ERT2CreERT2 and pCALNL-DsRed. F.O. is thankful to N. Osakada for constant encouragement and support. We are grateful for support from the National Institutes of Health (MH063912, NS069464, and EY010742: E.M.C.), the Kavli Institute for Brain and Mind at University of California San Diego (E.M.C.), the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (F.O.), the Kanae Foundation for the Promotion of Medical Science (F.O.), the Uehara Memorial Foundation (F.O.), and the Naito Foundation (F.O.). “
“Functional neural circuits consist of precise connectivity between specific sets of neurons. The assembly of such circuitry often requires that axons bypass numerous targets before

selectively terminating in just one or a few specific targets. Over the last century, much progress has been made in understanding however how axons undergo directed growth and pathfinding and how they form topographic maps (Sperry, 1963, Dickson, 2002 and Feldheim and O’Leary, 2010). How mammalian axons identify which targets to innervate, however, remains poorly understood. The axonal connections formed by the eyes with the brain are an attractive model for exploring mechanisms of axon-target recognition in the mammalian CNS. Retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) are the output neurons of the eye and they are divided into ∼20 different types. Each RGC type encodes a different quality of the visual scene, such as brightness, direction of motion or edges (Masland, 2001 and Berson, 2008), and sends that information to a limited number of retinorecipient targets that in turn regulate specific aspects of perception and behavior.

If thousands of variants confer susceptibility to MD, then this c

If thousands of variants confer susceptibility to MD, then this could explain a genetic correlation with other psychiatric disorders. We have no reason to expect the genetic architecture of anxiety, BP, or schizophrenia to be very different from MD: they are all likely to involve many loci of small Forskolin order effect, and they

are all, at some level, brain disorders. Indeed, Ripke and colleagues estimate that 8,300 independent SNPs contribute to the genetic basis of schizophrenia, accounting for 50% of the variance in liability to schizophrenia (Ripke et al., 2013a). With 18,000 genes expressed in the brain (Lein et al., 2007), and each disorder influenced by variants in thousands of genes, genetic correlation may be inevitable. The second point to note about the correlation between MD

and other disorders concerns how well selleck the phenotypic distinctions have been drawn. For example, no one has been able to identify features that distinguish with high accuracy episodes of MD in unipolar cases from episodes of MD in cases with bipolar illness. Furthermore, there is evidence that MD and BP share more characteristics than is sometimes appreciated: several authors have claimed that a large number of patients diagnosed with unipolar disorder have features of bipolar illness (Angst et al., 2010, Angst et al., 2011, Cassano et al., 2004 and Zimmermann et al., 2009). When symptoms of subthreshold mania are sought (elevated mood, irritable mood, or increased activity), a large proportion of unipolar cases are found to qualify: up

to half of all cases with unipolar illness (Angst et al., 2010, Angst et al., 2011 and Zimmermann et al., 2009). However, subthreshold diagnoses depend critically on the quality of the assessments and the exact interpretation of what constitutes subclinical mania (it is easy to confuse a state of hypomania with elation from “normal” causes like falling in love, or getting a grant funded in grim times, or hyperactivity from the agitation that occurs in some depressive subtypes). We can conclude that genetic and phenotypic classifications concur in identifying Phosphoprotein phosphatase considerable overlap between anxiety and MD, with mixed support for a distinction between MD and bipolar disorder. The genetic data point to genetic overlap, but this may be, to some extent, a consequence of the polygenicity of complex traits. We turn next to the question of whether there exists a pure MD, rarer and harder to distinguish from bipolar than currently acknowledged, which has at least partly distinct genetic roots. Or more generally, we ask, are there genetically homogenous subtypes of MD? Those unfamiliar with the literature debating the division of MD into subtypes may be surprised not only at the diversity of the proposed classificatory systems employed (e.g.

Analysis of the correlation coefficients of ensemble responses at

Analysis of the correlation coefficients of ensemble responses at different time points with the initial activity patterns (time average of 0.75–1.25 s Obeticholic Acid after odor

onset) confirmed that ensemble activity patterns became more decorrelated over time in the awake versus anesthetized state (Figure 2G). We performed principal component analysis to explore how the different temporal dynamics of odor responses in the awake and anesthetized state contributed to the ability of mitral cell ensembles to distinguish different odors over time. Representations of odor-evoked activity as a function of time in principal component space revealed that ensemble activity patterns for different odors were more separated in the awake state, and the separation in the awake state significantly improved over time (Figure 2H). This temporal improvement of odor classification in the awake state was confirmed when we considered the fractions of correctly classified trials, using responses in increasing time windows. Considering the numbers of

responses that gave the same levels of correct classification in awake and anesthetized states during the first second of odor stimulus (awake: 15 responses; SNS-032 mouse anesthetized: 25), we find that odor classification improved over the course of odor stimulation more strongly in the awake state (Figure 2I). Thus, the improvement of classification efficiency in the awake state is partly due to the odor-specific temporal dynamics. Taken together, these results indicate that odor representations in the awake state are more sparse, temporally dynamic, and efficient, compared to anesthetized brain states. Granule cells the are a major class of GABAergic interneurons in the olfactory bulb that mediate mitral cell recurrent and lateral inhibition via dendrodendritic synapses (Isaacson and Strowbridge, 1998; Schoppa et al., 1998; Yokoi et al., 1995). Therefore, we next considered the possibility

that differences in mitral cell odor representations between awake and anesthetized states could reflect differences in granule cell activity. We expressed GCaMP3 by injecting a nonconditional viral vector in the olfactory bulb granule cell layer of wild-type mice. Several weeks after injection, dense sets of neurons in the granule cell layer were visible through a cranial window (Figure 3A). While the vast majority of neurons in this layer are granule cells, our sample probably contains a small fraction of short axon cells, a heterogeneous class of interneurons in the granule cell layer (Eyre et al., 2008; Pressler and Strowbridge, 2006). In awake mice, a large fraction of granule cells showed spontaneous increases in GCaMP fluorescence that are sometimes temporally correlated to one another (Figure 3B, top). Similar to mitral cells, odors activated ensembles of granule cells in awake mice (Figures 3C and 3D).

74 and 0 35 for monkey M1 and M2, respectively (M1: p = 0 001; M2

74 and 0.35 for monkey M1 and M2, respectively (M1: p = 0.001; M2: p = 0.15; Fisher Z test; see Figure S4). Across all 34 3D-structure-selective sites, 22 sites (65%) contained at least one electrode position for which the MUA was significantly 3D-structure-selective buy MK0683 at each position in depth (p < 0.05, t test). Ten (75%)

of the remaining 12 sites contained at least one electrode position for which the MUA was significantly 3D-structure-selective for two positions in depth (p < 0.05, t test). In none of the 3D-structure-selective sites did we observe a significant reversal in structure preference at any position-in-depth (p > 0.05, t test). Hence, all 3D-structure-selective sites were characterized by only one 3D-structure preference. We tested whether stimulation in clusters containing MUA positions with significant selectivity for all positions-in-depth (putative completely invariant sites) caused larger microstimulation effects compared to stimulation in clusters with MUA positions that did not display significant structure selectivity at each position in depth (putative incompletely invariant sites). Stimulation in clusters with completely

invariant MUA positions caused significantly larger microstimulation Baf-A1 in vivo effects in monkey M1 (mean psychometric shift of 45% versus 19%; p = 0.005) but not in monkey M2 (mean psychometric shift of 12% versus 9%; p > 0.05), although a trend was and present. Given that we probably did not only stimulate completely invariant cells and given

the consistency of the microstimulation results, even in clusters with incomplete invariance (p < 0.003 for each monkey; t test for a significant psychometric shift toward more preferred choices), it seems possible that 3D-structure categorization does not solely rely on IT cells with complete tolerance for position-in-depth. Yet we cannot exclude the possibility that stimulation in 3D-structure selective clusters with incomplete invariance may have also stimulated nearby completely invariant structure-selective cells, from which we did not record, that caused the increase in preferred choices. Considering only the trials in which monkeys made a preferred choice, we observed significantly shorter average reaction times on stimulated compared to nonstimulated trials (Figures 6A and 6C; M1: average RT-difference: 3 ms; p = 0.04; M2: average RT-difference: 11 ms; p = 0.006; ANOVA). Furthermore, for non-preferred-choice trials, we noticed significantly longer average reaction times on stimulated compared to nonstimulated trials (Figures 6B and 6D; M1: average RT-difference: ∼5 ms; p = 0.002; M2: average RT-difference: ∼17 ms; p = 0.003; ANOVA).

4 kg which is similar to values reported in previous studies with

4 kg which is similar to values reported in previous studies with 12–16 weeks of 1-h twice-weekly recreational soccer training for untrained women13, 14, 15, 16 and 17 Adriamycin solubility dmso even though the training volume was only a quarter of that in the other studies (30 vs. 120 min/week). The potential clinical significance of reducing abdominal fat has been highlighted by studies such

as Rexrode et al. 36 who have reported a higher abdominal adiposity being associated with an increased risk of coronary heart disease in a cohort of 44,702 female registered nurses aged 40–65. The estimated energy consumption over 8 h of soccer training (30 min/week over 16 weeks) at an average HR of 155 bpm for untrained women would be in the area of 4000 kcal, corresponding to about 0.5 kg of fat. 9 It may therefore be speculated that fat oxidation was elevated outside the soccer training, as was found in other studies 17 which demonstrated a

positive effect on cardiovascular and metabolic fitness after 12–16 weeks of recreational soccer training, where an increase in the fat oxidation capacity at low to moderate exercise intensities, corresponding to the intensity during everyday life activity, was observed. That no equivalent overall group fat losses were seen following WBV training may well be due to the absence of an equivalent raising of the HR as observed during the soccer training. Other studies using oscillating 27 and 28 and vertical 37 WBV training have similarly reported no alterations in fat mass. Although WBV training has been reported to stimulate muscular work and to elevate metabolic rate to some extent, 38 the stimulus is probably insufficient to check details cause any change in fat mass for inactive premenopausal women. After 16-week of soccer training, the HR was on average 10 bpm lower in the last phase of a standardised submaximal YYIE1 test. A drop in HR loading during submaximal exercise indicates an increase in aerobic fitness and

is in accordance with findings from previous studies using recreational soccer training for premenopausal women. HR was found to decrease by 10–20 bpm during walking and jogging at 6–11 km/h after 16 weeks of twice-weekly 1-h soccer sessions for 20–45-year-old untrained women in conjunction with an increase in maximal oxygen uptake of 15%,13 and 17 and HR decreased by 7 bpm during DNA ligase submaximal cycling exercise after 12 weeks of training for twice-weekly 1-h soccer sessions for 25–65-year-old women, who had an increase in maximal oxygen uptake of 5% over the course of training.14 The present study also indicated positive effects on muscular aerobic fitness for the SG in comparison to the VG and the CO. The suggestion of an improvement in oxidative metabolism for the SG is reinforced by the recorded decrease in PCr depletion at the end of the ramp test at an equivalent time point, after the training intervention compared with the pre-training value.

The tonotopic maps run in a caudorostral direction and show mirro

The tonotopic maps run in a caudorostral direction and show mirror reversals in their preferred frequency gradient at the points of their lowest and highest frequencies. With three chronically implanted μECoG arrays (total 96 channels; Figures 1B and 1C), we were able to characterize these multiple maps simultaneously by measuring responses

to each stimulus presentation. We were also able to measure spontaneous activity from the same cortical positions in a separate testing session. Furthermore, the simultaneous recording with chronic μECoG arrays permitted the analysis of spatiotemporal covariation in the spontaneous field potentials. In the following sections we first MK-8776 cost describe the methods that identified the tonotopic maps in the auditory cortex and then demonstrate that spontaneous activity

is organized in a manner that reflects the specific structure of these maps. We recorded auditory evoked potentials from the implanted μECoG arrays while the monkeys listened passively to 180 different pure tone stimuli (each 100 ms duration, at 30 different frequencies from 100 Hz to 20 kHz at each of 6 intensity levels from 52–87 dB; see Experimental Procedures). The tone stimuli evoked robust responses (Figure 2). Unlike the average evoked waveform, which was dominated by the lower frequency components phase-locked to stimulus onset BIBW2992 price (Figure 2A, lower panels), the normalized spectrogram clearly shows increases

from the prestimulus period in higher frequency power, especially in the high gamma range (60–250 Hz; Figure 2A, upper panels) possibly due to the fact that the spectrogram reflects not only phase-locked Idoxuridine but also non-phase-locked components of the evoked response. To evaluate the auditory frequency preferences of individual sites, we estimated the characteristic frequency (CF) for each site based on the evoked high gamma power from the first 150 ms after the presentation of the stimulus (see Experimental Procedures). This produced tuning profiles with clear frequency preferences, which sometimes became better defined at lower sound intensities (Figure 2B, left; see also Figure S3, available online) but could also be relatively independent of sound intensity (Figure 2B, right). The statistical significance of this frequency preference was evaluated using a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA, p < 0.01; see Experimental Procedures). About two-thirds of the sites in STP showed significant frequency tuning (65/96 sites in monkey M, 62/96 sites in monkey B) and were organized in a tonotopic fashion, with CF reversals (Figure 3). The maps were similar in the two monkeys. Starting at the most caudal electrodes in the caudal-most array, sites were tuned to comparatively high frequencies. Moving rostrally along the STP, the frequency tuning went through at least three well-defined reversals over a distance of roughly 2 cm.

Importantly, when perception was studied with a protocol designed

Importantly, when perception was studied with a protocol designed to minimize the influence of learning and memory, monkeys with perirhinal lesions performed normally, even on very difficult discriminations where the stimuli

were rotated, enlarged, shrunk, desaturated, or degraded by masks (Hampton and Murray, 2002). In the present study we developed a tactic to reduce the possible influence of learning and memory impairment on perceptual performance. Rather than buy Enzalutamide train animals to learn many discriminations and then present single probe trials for each discrimination (Hampton and Murray, 2002), we trained animals to learn a single discrimination and then, while maintaining a high level of performance, presented 150 probe trials at each of 14 different levels of feature ambiguity. We suggest that rats with perirhinal cortex lesions exhibited intact performance on every probe trial level because performance did not require any new learning. The GDC-0068 manufacturer basic discrimination was very well learned and performance remained high throughout testing. One study with rats deserves mention (Bartko et al., 2007). Lego blocks were used to construct sets of objects with different levels of feature overlap (four levels were used). By using an exploratory task in which rats prefer to explore the odd object in a group of three (with all objects available at the same time), rats

with perirhinal cortex lesions performed normally when the objects were most distinct Parvulin but were impaired when the objects had high degrees of feature overlap. Yet as noted previously (Suzuki, 2009), it is possible that rats must hold objects in memory as they move back and forth examining the different objects. In support of this idea, a related study found that rats with perirhinal lesions did exhibit impaired performance on this task but that rats with hippocampal lesions exhibited the same pattern of impairment (N.J. Broadbent et al., 2009, Soc. Neurosci., abstract). These findings raise the possibility that impaired

performance on this task might reflect impaired learning and memory rather than impaired perception. Studies with feature-ambiguous stimuli have also been carried out with patients who have medial temporal lobe damage that includes the perirhinal cortex (Lee et al., 2005, Barense et al., 2007 and Lee and Rudebeck, 2010). Yet attempts to replicate some of this work and to find impairments with new tests were not successful (Kim et al., 2011 and Shrager et al., 2006). We (Squire and Wixted, 2011) and others (Suzuki, 2009 and Suzuki, 2010) have suggested that patients with perirhinal damage who exhibit impaired performance on tasks of visual perception may have significant additional damage to the adjacent lateral temporal cortex. In summary, we have demonstrated that the capacity to resolve feature ambiguity can be systematically studied in the rat with considerable rigor.

Retinal ganglion cells were analyzed in term of contrast sensitiv

Retinal ganglion cells were analyzed in term of contrast sensitivity, only. For this purpose we designed three different protocols of stimulation. Luminance (irradiance) sensitivity was assessed by stimulating the dark-adapted fish with a series of flashes (4 × 3 s flashes at 6 s intervals) at nine different light intensities, ranging between 11 pW/mm2 and 110 nW/mm2 with 0.5 log unit steps. The sequence of light intensities was randomized to reduce habituation artifacts in the recordings. Maximum light intensity, 110 nW/mm2, is equivalent to 3.3 × 1011 photons/mm2 × s−1. Contrast sensitivity AZD9291 manufacturer was assessed by stimulating the dark-adapted fish with a series

of 10 s light oscillations at 5 Hz around a constant light level (55 nW/mm2) at 10 different levels of contrast, ranging from 10% to 100% of the constant light level. Finally, frequency sensitivity was assessed by stimulating the dark-adapted fish with a series of 10 s light oscillations around a constant light level (55 nW/mm2) at 90% contrast at 14 different frequencies, ranging from 0.2 to 25 Hz. Image sequences were acquired at 10 Hz (256 × 100 pixels per frame, 1 ms per line) for the irradiance and contrast experiments and at 40 Hz

(256 × 25 pixels per frame, 1 ms per line) for frequency experiments. The stimulation of the olfactory bulb was obtained by bath application of the amino acid methionine (Sigma) 1 mM, as in Maaswinkel and Li (2003). To manipulate Crenolanib dopamine signaling in the retina we injected neuroactive drugs into the the eye. Final concentrations of the drugs were calculated by diluting the injected concentration into the free volume of the eye. The volume of a typical 9 dpf old zebrafish eye was assessed by three-dimensional reconstruction of the eye chamber and the lens through two-photon microscopy scanning. The final volume was estimated as the

difference between the total eye volume and the volume of the lens core. We calculated a total free volume of ∼500 μm3. Given a typical injected volume of 10 μl, the final dilution factor can be approximated to 1:50. Dopamine receptors were activated by injection of the long-lasting dopamine receptor ligand [3H] 2-amino-6,7-dihydroxy 1,2,3,4-tetrahydronapthalene (ADTN) (Sigma) 10 μM, as in Li and Dowling (2000b). Dopamine action on postsynaptic targets was prevented by injection of the strong dopamine D1 receptor antagonist SCH 23390 (Sigma) 2 nM, as in Huang et al. (2005) or the selective dopamine D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride (Sigma), as in Lin and Yazulla (1994) and Mora-Ferrer and Gangluff (2000). Finally, the level of dopamine in the circuit was frozen by injection of the dopamine release and reuptake inhibitor vanoxerine (Santa Cruz Biotechnology) 2 μM, as in Schlicker et al. (1996).